Representational Similarity Analysis

Overview and Considerations for MEG

September 27, 2016

Description


Motivation


MEG Implementation


Examples

WHAT IS IT?

RSA

Kriegeskorte et al., 2008
Neuroscientific theory must abstract from the idiosyncrasies of particular empirical modalities. To this end, we need a modality-independent way of characterizing a brain region’s representation. ... One way of characterizing the information a brain region represents is in terms of the mental states (e.g., stimulus percepts) it distinguishes.

RSA

  • Interpret brain data pattern as representation of stimulus
  • Compare those patterns between pairs of stimuli
  • Inference based on comparisons of dissimilarity
  • Where else are these conditions different in the same way
    • Between brain regions
    • Between model and brain data
    • Between subjects

RSA

Kriegeskorte & Kievit, 2013: Box 1 Figure 1

Representational Dissimilarity Matrix

  • Each cell shows dissimilarity between two items
    • Usually correlation distance ($1-r$)
    • Distance metric matters (we'll return to this)
  • Symmetric about diagonal of zeros
Kriegeskorte et al., 2008: Fig. 10

RDMs

We can also build RDMs from:

Models

  • Categorical (similar or not?)
  • Feature-vectors
  • Computational models
  • One-at-a-time variables (magnitude difference)

Behavioral Data

  • Similarity judgments
  • RT / Accuracies
  • Quantitative relationships between stimuli
  • Modality-independent
Kriegeskorte et al., 2008: Fig. 3A

Comparing RDMs

  • RDMs can be quantitatively compared with the same distance metrics
  • Use only upper (or lower) triangle of each RDM ($\binom{N}{2}$ unique pairs)
  • Calculate correlation between them (Pearson, Spearman, etc.)

Significance Testing

RDM Permutation

  • Permute columns of one RDM; recalculate correlation

Bootstrap

  • Remake both RDMs by sampling (with replacement) original conditions; recalculate correlation

Permutation Cluster Tests

  • (Spatio)temporal clusters where correlation > 0 across group

WHAT'S IT GOOD FOR?

Common Ground

Interface for any modality assuming same stimulus set Kriegeskorte et al., 2008: Fig. 3B

Visualization with MDS

Dimensionality reduction to view relationships in 2D

Within-RDM Kriegeskorte et al., 2008: Fig. 2 (partial)
Between-RDM Kriegeskorte et al., 2008: Fig. 9B

Not good for...

  • ...linear modeling of brain activity.
  • ...exploratory designs.
  • ...testing many continuous variables.

Good for...

  • ...experiments with strong priors about how stimuli/conditions will pattern.
  • ...comparing specific theoretical/computational accounts.
  • ...identifying neural correlates of behavioral effects.

Pattern-sensitivity

Kriegeskorte & Kievit, 2013: Figure 1

Pattern insensitivity

  • Signals can be perfectly correlated and have different magnitudes
  • Not "dissimilar" according to $1-r$
  • Need other distance measure (Euclidean, etc.)

HOW DO WE USE IT?

ROI: Temporal Searchlight

  • Time series of RDMs
  • Multivariate! Use all sources in ROI instead of averaging
  • Sliding time window
  • At each timepoint:
    • By-item vectors with length (sources x window size)
    • Build data RDM
    • Calculate correlation with model RDM
Tyler et al., 2013: Fig. 6B

Spatiotemporal Searchlight

  • RDMs over space and time
  • Data within spatial radius and temporal window of source-timepoint
  • Spatiotemporal map of model-brain correlations

Significance Testing

    Random-effects

    • Permute sign of correlation 10,000 times; build null distribution of largest clusters
    • (Su et al., 2012; Tyler et al., 2013)

    Fixed-effects

    • Permute data RDM columns at each (source-)timepoint
    • Recalculate correlations; build null distribution of largest clusters
    • Needs single trials

EXAMPLE

Example with MNE sample data